Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance? (2024)

Abstract

1.With increasing numbers of many herbivorous waterfowl species, often foraging on farmland, the conflict with agriculture has intensified. One popular management tool is to scare birds off the land, often in association with shooting. However, the energy costs of flying are considerably higher than those of resting. Therefore, when birds fly off after a disturbance, they use extra energy that subsequently needs to be compensated.

2.We used the white-fronted goose Anser albifrons, the most common (grass-eating) species wintering in western Europe, as a model species. We measured flight durations by high-frequency accelerometer recordings over 2 × 24 h in 9 focal geese that were only incidentally disturbed. We also made direct observations on these days to determine whether the flight durations were reliably recorded. Using both a simple and a more realistic model of the energy balance, we calculated the extra grass consumption resulting from additional intentional disturbances.

3.On average, the geese flew daily 2 × 323 s (from and to their roosting sites at 3200 m), and furthermore took to the air 5.3 times during a day (and 1.9 times a night). Multiplied with the average flight durations of 195 s, this gives a total flying time of almost 0.6 h day-1 and a total foraging time of 7.4 h day-1. The extra foraging time needed to compensate for additional intentional disturbances strongly depends on the frequency of such disturbances and the following flight duration. If, for example, flights when intentionally disturbed are twice as long (2 × 195 s), the extra foraging time will be 3.7% day-1 (2.3–3.2% day-1 in the more realistic model) for each intentional disturbance, and the geese will no longer be able to cover their energy requirements when intentionally disturbed six times per day.

4.Synthesis and applications. Recent experiments suggest that geese have to be scared frequently in order to reduce goose visitation to particular fields. With an intentional disturbance rate e.g. of five times a day, the birds’ compensation for the increased energy expenditure will lead to a higher overall consumption of grass of 11.5–16 % day-1. Accommodation schemes have to take such increases in total grass consumption into account when deciding on the refuge areas to be set aside.

Original languageEnglish
Pages (from-to)1413-1421
JournalJournal of Applied Ecology
Volume53
Early online date16 May 2016
DOIs
Publication statusPublished - 2016

Keywords

  • international

Access to Document

  • 6071_NoletFinal published version, 2.14 MBLicence: CC BY-NC-ND

Other files and links

    Fingerprint

    Dive into the research topics of 'Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.

    View full fingerprint

    • Data from: Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance[tracking data]

      Kölzsch, A. (Creator), Müskens, G. J. D. M. (Creator), Nolet, B. A. (Creator) & Wikelski, M. (Creator), Movebank, 2016

      DOI: doi:10.5441/001/1.7tp81b7b , https://www.datarepository.movebank.org/handle/10255/move.545

      Dataset

    • Data from: Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?[http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n4m05]

      Nolet, B. A. (Creator), Kölzsch, A. (Creator), Elderenbosch, M. (Creator) & Van Noordwijk, A. J. (Creator), Dryad, 31 May 2016

      DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n4m05

      Dataset

    • Data from: Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?[10.5441/001/1.7tp81b7b]

      Kölzsch, A. (Creator), Müskens, G. J. D. M. (Creator), Nolet, B. A. (Creator) & Wikelski, M. (Creator), Movebank, 2016

      DOI: doi:10.5441/001/1.7tp81b7b

      Dataset

    Cite this

    • APA
    • Author
    • BIBTEX
    • Harvard
    • Standard
    • RIS
    • Vancouver

    Nolet, B. A., Kölzsch, A., Elderenbosch, M., & Van Noordwijk, A. J. (2016). Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance? Journal of Applied Ecology, 53, 1413-1421. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.12698

    Nolet, B.A. ; Kölzsch, A. ; Elderenbosch, M. et al. / Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?. In: Journal of Applied Ecology. 2016 ; Vol. 53. pp. 1413-1421.

    @article{477adde5a3f14bcf8cf3c34487fbc3c0,

    title = "Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?",

    abstract = "1.With increasing numbers of many herbivorous waterfowl species, often foraging on farmland, the conflict with agriculture has intensified. One popular management tool is to scare birds off the land, often in association with shooting. However, the energy costs of flying are considerably higher than those of resting. Therefore, when birds fly off after a disturbance, they use extra energy that subsequently needs to be compensated.2.We used the white-fronted goose Anser albifrons, the most common (grass-eating) species wintering in western Europe, as a model species. We measured flight durations by high-frequency accelerometer recordings over 2 × 24 h in 9 focal geese that were only incidentally disturbed. We also made direct observations on these days to determine whether the flight durations were reliably recorded. Using both a simple and a more realistic model of the energy balance, we calculated the extra grass consumption resulting from additional intentional disturbances.3.On average, the geese flew daily 2 × 323 s (from and to their roosting sites at 3200 m), and furthermore took to the air 5.3 times during a day (and 1.9 times a night). Multiplied with the average flight durations of 195 s, this gives a total flying time of almost 0.6 h day-1 and a total foraging time of 7.4 h day-1. The extra foraging time needed to compensate for additional intentional disturbances strongly depends on the frequency of such disturbances and the following flight duration. If, for example, flights when intentionally disturbed are twice as long (2 × 195 s), the extra foraging time will be 3.7% day-1 (2.3–3.2% day-1 in the more realistic model) for each intentional disturbance, and the geese will no longer be able to cover their energy requirements when intentionally disturbed six times per day.4.Synthesis and applications. Recent experiments suggest that geese have to be scared frequently in order to reduce goose visitation to particular fields. With an intentional disturbance rate e.g. of five times a day, the birds{\textquoteright} compensation for the increased energy expenditure will lead to a higher overall consumption of grass of 11.5–16 % day-1. Accommodation schemes have to take such increases in total grass consumption into account when deciding on the refuge areas to be set aside.",

    keywords = "international",

    author = "B.A. Nolet and A. K{\"o}lzsch and M. Elderenbosch and {Van Noordwijk}, A.J.",

    note = "6071, AnE; Data archiving: data archived at Movebank and at Dryad",

    year = "2016",

    doi = "10.1111/1365-2664.12698",

    language = "English",

    volume = "53",

    pages = "1413--1421",

    journal = "Journal of Applied Ecology",

    issn = "0021-8901",

    publisher = "John Wiley and Sons Ltd",

    }

    Nolet, BA, Kölzsch, A, Elderenbosch, M & Van Noordwijk, AJ 2016, 'Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?', Journal of Applied Ecology, vol. 53, pp. 1413-1421. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.12698

    Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance? / Nolet, B.A.; Kölzsch, A.; Elderenbosch, M. et al.
    In: Journal of Applied Ecology, Vol. 53, 2016, p. 1413-1421.

    Research output: Contribution to journal/periodicalArticleScientificpeer-review

    TY - JOUR

    T1 - Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance?

    AU - Nolet, B.A.

    AU - Kölzsch, A.

    AU - Elderenbosch, M.

    AU - Van Noordwijk, A.J.

    N1 - 6071, AnE; Data archiving: data archived at Movebank and at Dryad

    PY - 2016

    Y1 - 2016

    N2 - 1.With increasing numbers of many herbivorous waterfowl species, often foraging on farmland, the conflict with agriculture has intensified. One popular management tool is to scare birds off the land, often in association with shooting. However, the energy costs of flying are considerably higher than those of resting. Therefore, when birds fly off after a disturbance, they use extra energy that subsequently needs to be compensated.2.We used the white-fronted goose Anser albifrons, the most common (grass-eating) species wintering in western Europe, as a model species. We measured flight durations by high-frequency accelerometer recordings over 2 × 24 h in 9 focal geese that were only incidentally disturbed. We also made direct observations on these days to determine whether the flight durations were reliably recorded. Using both a simple and a more realistic model of the energy balance, we calculated the extra grass consumption resulting from additional intentional disturbances.3.On average, the geese flew daily 2 × 323 s (from and to their roosting sites at 3200 m), and furthermore took to the air 5.3 times during a day (and 1.9 times a night). Multiplied with the average flight durations of 195 s, this gives a total flying time of almost 0.6 h day-1 and a total foraging time of 7.4 h day-1. The extra foraging time needed to compensate for additional intentional disturbances strongly depends on the frequency of such disturbances and the following flight duration. If, for example, flights when intentionally disturbed are twice as long (2 × 195 s), the extra foraging time will be 3.7% day-1 (2.3–3.2% day-1 in the more realistic model) for each intentional disturbance, and the geese will no longer be able to cover their energy requirements when intentionally disturbed six times per day.4.Synthesis and applications. Recent experiments suggest that geese have to be scared frequently in order to reduce goose visitation to particular fields. With an intentional disturbance rate e.g. of five times a day, the birds’ compensation for the increased energy expenditure will lead to a higher overall consumption of grass of 11.5–16 % day-1. Accommodation schemes have to take such increases in total grass consumption into account when deciding on the refuge areas to be set aside.

    AB - 1.With increasing numbers of many herbivorous waterfowl species, often foraging on farmland, the conflict with agriculture has intensified. One popular management tool is to scare birds off the land, often in association with shooting. However, the energy costs of flying are considerably higher than those of resting. Therefore, when birds fly off after a disturbance, they use extra energy that subsequently needs to be compensated.2.We used the white-fronted goose Anser albifrons, the most common (grass-eating) species wintering in western Europe, as a model species. We measured flight durations by high-frequency accelerometer recordings over 2 × 24 h in 9 focal geese that were only incidentally disturbed. We also made direct observations on these days to determine whether the flight durations were reliably recorded. Using both a simple and a more realistic model of the energy balance, we calculated the extra grass consumption resulting from additional intentional disturbances.3.On average, the geese flew daily 2 × 323 s (from and to their roosting sites at 3200 m), and furthermore took to the air 5.3 times during a day (and 1.9 times a night). Multiplied with the average flight durations of 195 s, this gives a total flying time of almost 0.6 h day-1 and a total foraging time of 7.4 h day-1. The extra foraging time needed to compensate for additional intentional disturbances strongly depends on the frequency of such disturbances and the following flight duration. If, for example, flights when intentionally disturbed are twice as long (2 × 195 s), the extra foraging time will be 3.7% day-1 (2.3–3.2% day-1 in the more realistic model) for each intentional disturbance, and the geese will no longer be able to cover their energy requirements when intentionally disturbed six times per day.4.Synthesis and applications. Recent experiments suggest that geese have to be scared frequently in order to reduce goose visitation to particular fields. With an intentional disturbance rate e.g. of five times a day, the birds’ compensation for the increased energy expenditure will lead to a higher overall consumption of grass of 11.5–16 % day-1. Accommodation schemes have to take such increases in total grass consumption into account when deciding on the refuge areas to be set aside.

    KW - international

    UR - http://dx.doi.org/10.5441/001/1.7tp81b7b

    UR - http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n4m05

    U2 - 10.1111/1365-2664.12698

    DO - 10.1111/1365-2664.12698

    M3 - Article

    SN - 0021-8901

    VL - 53

    SP - 1413

    EP - 1421

    JO - Journal of Applied Ecology

    JF - Journal of Applied Ecology

    ER -

    Nolet BA, Kölzsch A, Elderenbosch M, Van Noordwijk AJ. Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance? Journal of Applied Ecology. 2016;53:1413-1421. Epub 2016 May 16. doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.12698

    Scaring waterfowl as a management tool: how much more do geese forage after disturbance? (2024)
    Top Articles
    Latest Posts
    Article information

    Author: Msgr. Benton Quitzon

    Last Updated:

    Views: 5336

    Rating: 4.2 / 5 (63 voted)

    Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

    Author information

    Name: Msgr. Benton Quitzon

    Birthday: 2001-08-13

    Address: 96487 Kris Cliff, Teresiafurt, WI 95201

    Phone: +9418513585781

    Job: Senior Designer

    Hobby: Calligraphy, Rowing, Vacation, Geocaching, Web surfing, Electronics, Electronics

    Introduction: My name is Msgr. Benton Quitzon, I am a comfortable, charming, thankful, happy, adventurous, handsome, precious person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.